I’ve spent a fair bit of time trying to explain various psychological concepts in a way that is easily grasped by busy teachers and have come to the conclusion that some of my explanations might be worth recording on the blog.
First up is a simple explanation of what a schema is, how it is formed and why this is worth knowing.
Because we have no capacity to introspect our long-term memories no one has any idea what actually happens in there. We know we must have a long-term memory because we can think about something, stop thinking about it and think about it again later. Where does it go in between? We have no idea but this is what we call memory.
A schema is a theoretical construct that is no doubt wrong, but is, hopefully, useful. It can be thought of as an interconnected web of items of knowledge. The ability to retrieve items in memory is dependent on environmental cues and prompts. If I say bread, you say… * The fact that a prompt cues us to retrieve some connected information provides with some logical evidence that schema exist and that we store items we know to be connected together.
Let’s imagine you know only one solitary thing about a subject. This single item is represented by the green dot below. Because it’s not linked to anything else it will be very hard to retrieve. We would have to stumble upon precisely the correct prompt to be able to recall it.
Let’s now imagine that this solitary, unconnected fact is some vocabulary from a foreign language: the phrase ni hao. If this phrase is new to you, it will now be stored somewhere in your long-term memory but will, in all probability, be very hard to recall later. However, if you’ve encountered it before, maybe you remember what it means. Maybe you know it’s Mandarin for ‘hello’. If you do, then the Mandarin phase and its English meaning are stored as two, connected pieces of knowledge:
When I say ni hao, you say ‘hello’. This doubles your chances of retrieving either the Mandarin phrase or its English meaning.
The more Mandarin vocabulary you learn, the easier it becomes to recall every other item within your Mandarin schema:
This is one of the counter-intuitive things about schema – the more items and the greater the number of connections between items, the easier it becomes to draw the entire schema into working memory.
Eventually, you arrive at a point where it becomes automatic and effortless to recall most of the items within a schema and the whole network is thoroughly embedded with other, connected schemas.
If all the dots with the schema represented Mandarin vocabulary, we could, at this stage, say a person was a fluent Mandarin speaker. It turns out that fluent Mandarin speakers never forget how to say ni hao. They don’t bump into acquaintances, shrug sheepishly and say something along the lines of “I’m sorry, but there’s this way of greeting people but I just can’t for the life of me remember what it is.”
This is what it means to be fluent at anything: our ability to draw vast schemas into working memory effortless and automatically means we’re unaware of just how much we know. This is the same for times table knowledge, number bonds, items of grammar, European capital cities, a basic chronology of British history, the ability to drive, or whatever else we might need to effortlessly recall.
If we accept that learning (whatever else it is) is the acquisition of increasingly robust, interconnected schema, and that our role as teachers is to support children in being able to retrieve schemas effortlessly and automatically, it might make everyone’s job a tiny bit easier.
* Most people will say butter, some toast etc. Basically, anything that’s semantically linked in memory.
[…] don’t know exactly what would be the long-term memory equivalent of a gene. Would it be a schema? One of Ohlsson’s theories? I’m not sure. However, I don’t think it matters in […]
[…] future topics that link with the current topic. This also helps pupils build organised schema. See this by @DavidDidau for more on […]
Thank you for this methodical walkthrough. I have used it on some fellow colleagues as well as some students who were interested in how language acquisition works and it seemed as if it hit its mark (although, of course we cannot at this stage be sure, but still). Any advice on what one can do to influence fellow teachers who still hold on very stubbornly to old convictions such as e.g. learning styles and multiple intelligences? If I find the opportunity, I try to go about, using socratic dialogue, which to some extent seems to work.
Even so, the problem is that as you well know, there are rarely such opportunities which present themselves as we are busy teachers. How would you go about convincing colleagues in this matter? Mostly, I do not invest too much time or effort into it, but now, as the initiative is to work with an evidence-informed approach at work, it seems rather pertinent.
[…] of a schema – a web of knowledge that connects what we know to other things that we know. This short post by David Didau explains this idea of the schema and its implications for us as […]
[…] ‘How to explain…schema’ by David Didau is David doing what he has been doing brilliantly for years…writing really useful and thought provoking blogs. […]
[…] How to explain… schema 31st […]
[…] I spend sometime discussing the idea of schema – how tiny items of knowledge connect together to allows us to overcome the limits of our minds to hold on to new information and perform breathtaking feats of mental and physical agility. (I’ve explored this idea in some detail here.) […]
[…] they needed to make sense of the instruction and so are able to add new information to existing schemas with relative […]
Surely the inability to forget ‘ni hao’ is not to do with the schema but with the frequency with such a term would be used when attempting to converse? How do we know that it is a ‘web effect’ that enables recall rather than continued repetition and use of items of vocabulary?
Ah, the ‘surely klaxon’! The frequency with with something is recalled builds schematic strength. Each time we access an item in long-term memory, the we create new connections between that item and the context in which we retrieve it.
[…] our pupils’ long term memory is the idea of a ‘schema’. David Didau rightly points out in this post that this term isn’t a perfect way of explaining what’s going on but is still helpful for […]
[…] briefly summarise what I say in that post and what David Didau eloquently describes, when teachers attempt to explicitly help their pupils make links between the […]
[…] How to explain schema […]
[…] How to explain ‘schema’ […]
[…] written about how we can usefully think about and explain schema here. Now, these models are useful but they are wrong. The processes of memory are abstract and opaque; […]
[…] useful way of thinking about how this knowledge is stored in long-term memory is the construct of schema; an interconnected web of knowledge about a certain topic or concept. The more we learn about a […]