In the latest edition of the British Psychological Society’s Research Digest, Bradley Busch writes about a new study which compared the effects on memory of reading in silence to those of reading out loud. Noah Forrin and Colin MacLeod’s paper, This time it’s personal: the memory benefit of hearing oneself, explores what’s been termed the ‘production effect’ – a neat name for the memory advantage of saying words aloud over simply reading them silently. The speculation is that the effort of saying something out loud appears to make information more cognitively ‘sticky’, creating stronger schematic connections in long-term memory. This advantage appears to be strongest when we say things out loud ourselves, but also holds true when listening to someone else read aloud.
This has, I think, some important implications for two areas of classroom practice. Earlier this year I wrote about why there’s good reason to believe that asking students to listen to a text being read aloud whilst simultaneously expecting them to ‘follow along’ in silence is likely to over burden working memory and lead to poorer comprehension. I suggested that this was a form of multitasking which human beings are simply incapable of. Instead, we ‘task switch’, diverting attention from reading to listening, with a lag of a few seconds with every switch. My conclusion was as follows:
To be absolutely clear: reading aloud to students, especially weaker readers, is a good thing to do. Prosody – the sound and rhythm the words make – really aids comprehension, especially with difficult texts. The problem comes when children are expected to follow along at the same speed. Because they lack the fluency to do this their working memory overloads and derails comprehension.
Reading aloud to students – or even better, having them read aloud – is likely to also boost their memory. There are obvious classroom disadvantages to having every child read aloud, but asking the most skilled reader (probably the teacher) in the room to read aloud is likely to benefit everyone.
The second implication is around improving students’ writing. It’s been some years now since I first proposed my ‘simple theory of writing‘, but essentially what I suggest to teachers is that intervening at the point of writing is an ineffective way to improve writing. Intervening at the point of speech seems to be far more effective. Many students are unable to write down their thoughts because they simply don’t possess the knowledge of how to express their ideas in written language. If we scaffold students’ spoken language by making them ‘speak like an essay’ then my observation is that the act of speaking in academic language seems to strengthen the schema of how to communicate in academic language and thus writing becomes possible. My anecdotal finding has been that this is far more effective than using writing frames, so it’s pleasing to find some empirical support for the idea.
When I’m training teachers in effective instruction, one of the points I make is that if you want students to remember something, you have to make them say it aloud. If you just get one or two students to repeat something, then only one or two students will remember it. If one students finds a way to avoid having to speak, that’s the student who will be least likely to remember it. The answer is to use fast paced call and response whenever you want students to pay special attention to memorising a particular idea or phrase.
Of course, none of this is definitive proof that saying things out loud supports memory – the current research is based on participants studying word lists, so it will be important to see if the findings can be replicated with longer texts – but there’s a clear and plausible mechanism to explain why we should expect the production effect to be an effective way to teach and study.
The problem with academic language is that it’s not the same as normal spoken language. Even educated adults use a fairly restricted vocabulary in normal conversation, and of course we don’t always talk in grammatical sentences. With schoolchildren, this is even more apt to be true, which is why their writing is so often atrocious. It certainly helps to have parents whose speech is grammatically correct, and ones who use abstract words and words with Latin and Greek origins in normal speech. This is one ‘gap’ that almost defies closing, especially when teachers talk down to their pupils.
An additional handicap for most pupils is poor spelling–not only does this limit the use of academic words that the pupil can read and understand, but retrieval occupies so much working memory that it detracts from that available for sentence-level composition–never mind the structure and meaning of the essay.
There aren’t many easy answers, but in the spelling programmes I’ve written I always include a lot of sentence dictation exercises. The main purpose is to provide reinforcement of spellings which have been previously taught; with the additional load of recalling the words that have just been dictated, it’s easy to identify words and spelling patterns which need additional review. It also provides a model for a wide variety of grammatically-correct sentences–admittedly, I’ve never had a chance to see how much this transfers to other written work.
[…] https://www.learningspy.co.uk/reading/reading-aloud-might-boost-students-memories/ […]
[…] Reading aloud might boost students’ memories (The Learning Spy) The speculation is that the effort of saying something out loud appears to make information more cognitively ‘sticky’, creating stronger schematic connections in long-term memory. This advantages appears to be strongest when we say things out loud ourselves, but also holds true when listening to someone else read aloud. […]
Yes, completely on board with the “if they can’t say it they can’t write it” proposition. In science the precision of vocabulary and the structure of sentences is crucial, but really helpfully the structure is often repetitive e.g. “As x increases, y increases”. One of the things I find hard to get trainee teachers to appreciate is that children can often respond to questioning in a way that appears to demonstrate understanding but is actually very fragmented. Then what they write is garbage. Whereas if you get them to say the whole thing properly, their answers are much better. This is particularly effective in supporting lower achieving children to answer at a high level. Despite I think both explicitly saying this, and modelling it, I don’t see them doing it much in school. I think perhaps I’m not making a big enough thing of it, yet.
Thanks
[…] 13. Reading aloud might boost students’ memories, by David Didau […]
[…] Reading aloud might boost students’ memories – David Didau: The Learning Spy — Read on https://www.learningspy.co.uk/reading/reading-aloud-might-boost-students-memories/ […]