I slide I used in a presentation on the ideas in my book, Making Kids Cleverer has been getting a bit of love on Twitter, with New Zealand school principal referring to it as containing what might be “the three best sentences in education”.
This could be the three best sentences in education. Thanks @DavidDidau pic.twitter.com/cXn37GTeZG
— John Young (@JohnYoung18) December 9, 2020
Apart from the missing apostrophe in the second statement, this is obviously very gratifying, and I thought it would be useful to add some context and clarification.
The most advantaged will succeed despite what schools do.
This is a probabilistic statement. I’m not suggesting social advantage automatically confers academic success or that nothing that schools do makes any difference to the success of these children. Instead, what I’m suggesting is that because children from more socially advantaged backgrounds are also likely to come to school knowing more about the world, their academic success is less dependent on what happens in school. As a result, schools are systematically biased in favour of the most advantaged. We are geared up to give children from affluent backgrounds even more of what they come to us with, whereas children from disadvantaged backgrounds tend to be proportionately further disadvantaged by their encounters with education. This is the Matthew Effect. Often, we look at the progress data of students from affluent backgrounds and assume their success is down to us. We then use this to justify the efficacy or whatever it is we happen to believe is the best approach to education. However, if these students are statistically more likely to succeed regardless of what happens in school, then it might very well be true that their success is despite our actions.
To know if you’re successful, only look at data on the most disadvantaged students’ performance.
In contrast, children from less advantaged backgrounds are going to be more dependent on schools and teachers to be academically successful so we can be more certain that their success is due to our actions.Of course this is not always true – there are definitely children who are lucky in other ways – more intelligent, harder working, more resilient, etc. – but, again, this is a probabilistic claim. If we want to get a clearer sense of whether what we’re doing is ‘working’ then we need to look first at the performance of those who are disadvantaged. If there is a significant gap between the performance of the most advantaged and most disadvantaged students then we really shouldn’t be congratulating ourselves that our chosen approaches ‘works’ for those with most – their success may be in spite of us. However, if the most disadvantaged are doing as well as their more advantaged peers then that is likely to be because of our choices and actions. This way it is harder to fool ourselves.
What works best for the most disadvantaged students works best for all.
The strategies that are most likely to lead to disadvantaged students being academically successful are strategies that prioritise learning over current performance. We know that it’s perfectly possible for children to successfully solve problems in the classroom yet not remember how to solve them elsewhere or later. We know that children can expend their limited cognitive reserves completing engaging activities as well as on activities that contribute to building increasingly automatic schema. But if the tasks we give children are too complex they can’t do both at once. Similarly, we know that possessing culturally rich, shared knowledge makes it far more likely that children will be able to comprehend a wider range of reading material and therefore more likely to understand and be able to think creatively and critically about the information they encounter. Ensuring that all children are taught culturally rich shared knowledge using instructional practices that take into consideration the limitation of human thought and memory means that all children are more likely to be academically successful. Everybody benefits. But, because the most disadvantaged are more dependent on schools in order to get access to this knowledge, it disproportionately benefits those with least. We may never close the gap between those with most and those with least but we can at least prevent it from being widened further by students’ educational experiences.
First bold subheading – you mean most advantage right?
No. Just like the most disadvantaged people have a hard time in school, the most advantaged people in school have a better time in school.
If the advantaged start further ahead and what works best for the disadvantaged, works best for all, won’t this strategy at best maintain the gap, not close it?
It’s probably impossible to makes up for all of the advantages possessed by some students but the point of what I’m advocating is twofold:
1. Most approaches of education exacerbate the gap whereas this will not.
2. This approaches addresses the source of educational disadvantage – lack of knowledge – and expressly seeks to address it which is disproportionately beneficial to the least advantaged.
I do love the statements! Especially the last one: I think we should focus more on those students who need additional help in their studies
I agree with last of the 3 statements but here’s what really bothers me . Most data is taken from school testing which is in the written form. Increasingly, many disadvantaged pupils also have learning needs such as dyslexia or DLD and struggle to read, write or understand enough to complete the tests adequately. School relies solely on recording academic ability through being able to write in books. There’s a total loss of language development through simply talking, discussing, debating. Books sadly rule. Yet the majority of occupations in the real world have little to do with writing in books all day long. Why can’t we look at other ways to educate and truly support the disadvantaged in education. Why not introduce ‘ no pens day’ regularly throughout the year. ( communicationtrust.org)
I’ve written before about my reservations with initiatives like ‘no pens day’: https://learningspy.co.uk/literacy/negative-framing-pens-days/
Neglecting writing would not ‘support’ the most disadvantaged it would alienate them further. The attitude that ‘kids like these’ can’t read or will struggle to express themselves in writing is a wll-intentioned but sadly self-fulfilling prophecy.
[…] What works best for the most disadvantaged students works best for all. David Didau […]
I’d agree that those most advantaged by nature and nurture will more likely thrive. However, it isn’t clear to me that the methods to reach the most disadvantaged (again, disadvantaged by nature and/or nurture) will help speed average-advantaged kids on the way.
What, in your view, is the best way to teach advantaged children?
I’m still learning – as a retired exec who started a kids’ science program. I have a very rough model of five things kids need to become the best they can be. #2 of these is secure attachment – an adult who is on their side, providing support, and the sort of organized environment that leads kids to believe their own efforts matter. Fail to get that and you’re surely “disadvantaged.” It’s no longer just a problem of the poor.
#3 . . . I see lots of kids with that advantage of caring and competent adults in their lives but who are surrounded with so many options (toys, games, activities . . .) they move on to something else given the slightest obstacle. These can be bright and sometimes curious kids, with many material advantages — but without the grit, agency, self-motivation, determination (whatever various folks call it) to put in some hard work.
So, the question for me is if this self-motivation is something that is mostly innate or can it be taught? I expect – or at least hope – we can teach kids to persevere. One of the interesting lessons in business (at least to me) is that it takes about three times to get anything right whether it’s HP Laser Printers or Microsoft Windows software even if you’re a fast learner . . . a bit of Edison’s 99% perspiration to go along with inspiration. Similar story in basic research.
The notion (from your recently-read posters) of “cognitive load” seems a useful if vague heuristic. I try to find way cool activities to engage kids — with just-hard-enough tasks along the way that they’re likely to stumble — but rewarding, guided, and easy enough they pick themselves up and try again is an approach we’re trying. Not sure yet if it will work.
What do you think or maybe even know? Can we teach kids to be strongly and meaningfully self-motivated? If so, that might be what’s missing in life’s curriculum for our “advantaged” kids.